|
|
|
|
|
How
to Discriminate Against Women Without
Really Trying
by Jo
Freeman
Based
on research done in 1969 at the
University of Chicago, this paper was
published in Women: A
Feminist Perspective, first edition, 1975,
pp. 194-208; second
edition, 1979, pp. 217-232.
"Any
girl who gets this far has got to be
a kook," one distinguished (male)
member of the University of
Chicago faculty told a female graduate student
who had come to see him
about being on her dissertation committee.
This
was just one of many such statements
collected by women students at the
University in the Spring of 1969 to
illustrate their contention that "some
of our professors have
different expectations about our performance than
about the
performance of male graduate students— expectations based not
on our
ability as individuals but on the fact that we are women."
There
were many others. They included:
"The admissions committee didn't do their job. There is not one good
looking girl in the entering class."
"They've
been sending me too many women
advisees. I've got to do something about
that."
"You
have no business looking for work
with a child that age."
"I'm
sorry you lost your fellowship. You're getting married, aren't
you?"
"We
expect women who come here to be competent, good students; but we don't
expect them to be brilliant or original."
"I
see the number of women entering this
year has increased. I hope the quality
has increased as
well."
And
most
telling of all: "I know you're competent and your thesis advisor
knows you're competent. The question in our minds is are you really serious
about what you're doing." This was said to a young woman who had
already spent five years and over $10,000 getting to that point in her
Ph.D. program.
These
comments hardly contribute to a student's self-image as a scholar. Often
made in jest, they are typical of those used by professors on the University
of Chicago campus and other campuses to express the only socially
acceptable
prejudice left -- that against women. But if you were to
ask these same
professors whether they discriminate against women
students and colleagues,
most would answer that they do not.
Until
a few years ago, most women
would have agreed with them. Since then, many
of the women students
and faculty toward whom these comments were aimed
have looked at the
actions behind the words and concluded that most professors
discriminate against women whether they are conscious of it or not.
Women
in one social science department openly
declared that their professors
frequently discouraged them from going
to or staying in graduate school.
They said the attitude of their
professors indicated "that we are
expected to be decorative
objects in the classroom, that we're not likely
to finish a Ph.D. and
if we do, there must be something wrong with us."
They pointed
out that no woman had held a faculty position in their department
since the University was founded in 1892 and that this lack of role models
was hardly encouraging to women students.
At
the time the University community was
recovering from a massive sit-in
the previous quarter (winter 1969)
caused by the firing of Marlene Dixon,
the first woman to teach in the
Sociology Department in nineteen years.
Though women's issues were not
the primary concern of the protest, they
had been raised publicly for
the first time during the course of it, (thanks
largely to the efforts
of Dixon herself and of the campus women's liberation
group), and had
generated the greatest response from the University community.
One
such response was from the students, who
began to organize into departmental
caucuses, put out position papers,
and confront the faculty with their
new feminist consciousness. The
other response was from the faculty and
the administration. The
Committee of the Council of the Academic Senate
appointed a Committee
on University Women (COUW) to study "the situation
and
opportunities presently enjoyed by women in the University community."
The COUW created a student subcommittee (SCOUW) of six students and three
faculty members, of which I was chairperson.
|
|
|
THE
STUDY
As
part of its duties SCOUW developed a detailed, self-administered
questionnaire.
The design of the study1 involved
distributing the questionnaire
to a sample of approximately 50 male and
50 female respondents from
each of the seventeen graduate and undergraduate
divisions and
professional schools at the University of Chicago at the
beginning of
the following fall quarter (1969-70). These numbers were
chosen to
provide adequate samples for comparison. In the final tally
each
100-person unit was weighted to represent the actual relative strength
of its school or division in the University. The sampling intervals were
determined individually for each unit and sex, and the sample was drawn
by selecting every nth name from alphabetical lists of students registered
that spring.
Although
the
expected 30 percent loss rate was compensated for, the inevitable
elimination of the drop-outs in itself involves a bias: our study excluded
the University's casualties, who may well have been those most fruitful
to examine. This is analogous to studying the effects of a disease by
looking only at its survivors. Although the study no doubt included many
who would later drop out, the possibility of bias still exists. One could
maintain that those women who perceive discrimination most sharply or
get the least support from the University environment are the ones to
leave. To a certain extent we are dealing with a group of
self-selected
women who have at least partially adapted to the
system.
The
questionnaire took
about twenty minutes to complete as most of the questions
had precoded
answers from which the respondent picked one or more. Students
maintained their anonymity by turning in their completed questionnaires
separately from a card with their name on it. When a name card was received,
the respondent's name was removed from the sample list. Thus at any
given
moment we had a list of those who had not returned their
questionnaires
and who could be followed up by phone calls.
The
overall response rate was 77
percent. The loss was due partially to nonreturning
students and
partially to failure to turn in the questionnaires. Unfortunately,
the
rather low response rate was not the only problem the study ran into.
Other problems that could bias its results were encountered in three
spheres:
1. The Administration. The staff had received
assurances from intermediate
level administrators that if the
questionnaire was not offensive, it would
be distributed at
registration. Unfortunately, the Provost later decided
to avoid any
possible complications of the registration procedure and
rejected the
questionnaire without even looking at it. It might be noted
that it is
not uncommon for material to be distributed and studies undertaken
at
registration time. Only four years previously, permission had been
given for a study requiring every registering student to fill out a
questionnaire
on teeth-gnashing. Other studies using the registration
handout procedure
had been done in the interim. Our low response rate
can be attributed
largely to this administrative reversal, for the
officials of some of
the professional schools gave us full cooperation
and those units had
response rates near 90 percent.
2. The Faculty. The study was originally intended to be a far-ranging
one that would probe the causes of the problems women students faced in
general. Unfortunately, a majority of the faculty members of COUW and
SCOUW felt the survey should be used primarily to determine the extent
to which women perceived overt discrimination in the classroom and in
university services. It was felt that only items leading to explicit
recommendations
to the University should be included. For example,
questions on the amount
of time spent in child care were opposed on
the grounds that there was
nothing the University could do about any
sex differential that might
exist here. The Null Environment
Hypothesis which will be discussed in
detail below, was one of the few
nonspecific concerns to survive this
weeding out.
3. The Students. Members of the campus women's liberation group decided
to boycott the questionnaire for political reasons on which they did not
elaborate. Most of them were undergraduate women in the social sciences,
and this is where we found our lowest return rate -- 62 percent. Some
differences in return rates were certainly due to the different
distribution
and follow-up methods we had to use. However, the
procedures used in this
unit were identical for men and women, and the
men had a response rate
of 81 percent. There is good reason to
believe, therefore, that the boycott
was effective and that the
responses from this unit do not adequately
represent the militant
feminist viewpoint. Nonetheless, the results were
quite striking.
|
|
|
THE
NULL
ENVIRONMENT HYPOTHESIS
The
Null Environment Hypothesis was
a response to the contention of one of
the faculty members of SCOUW
that the faculty did not discriminate between
women and men students
-- they treated them all poorly. Succinctly put,
the hypothesis states
that an academic situation that neither encourages
nor discourages
students of either sex is inherently discriminatory against
women
because it fails to take into account the differentiating external
environments from which women and men students come. As Horner has pointed
out, many women enter school with a "motive to avoid success"
because they fear social rejection or find academic success in conflict
with a feminine identity.2 Even those
who have not internalized this notion often find little or no support
for intellectual aspirations -- particularly at the postgraduate level
-- because their endeavors are not taken seriously. Thus women enter
with
a handicap which a "null" academic environment does
nothing
to decrease and may well reinforce. In other words, professors
don't have
to make it a specific point to discourage their female
students. Society
will do that job for them. All they have to do is to
fail to encourage
them. Professors can discriminate against women
without really trying.
Obviously,
we first had to find out whether the hypothesized null environment
existed
at the University. Did the faculty provide a supportive
environment for
the students or did they not? Did they strongly
encourage them to develop
their potential or did they not?
We
asked students how they thought the faculty
felt about their going to
or being in graduate or professional school
and about their having a career.
We interpreted a response of
"very favorable" to mean that a
student felt he or she
received positive support. As can be seen in Tables
I and II, only 47
percent of the male students and 32 percent of the female
students
felt they got positive support or encouragement for postgraduate
education, either from the male 94 percent or the female 6 percent of
the faculty.3 Even fewer felt the
faculty were very favorable to their having a career. Thus if we define
a null academic environment as one with a significant lack of positive
support, we can say that such an environment did indeed exist at the
University
of Chicago. The lack of positive support for women was
especially evident.
Only a little over two-thirds as many female as
male students thought
the male faculty were "very favorable"
to their having advanced
education and a lesser proportion thought
they were favorable to their
having a career.
If
students do not receive support from
faculties for going on with their
education and careers, where do they
get it? Much of it comes from personal
commitment. The presence of
this internal support mechanism is always
assumed in men. Women are
supposed to be less deeply committed, and this
supposition is often
used both as an explanation of their lesser success
and as a
justification for preferential treatment for men. Ignoring for
the
moment the self-fulfilling prophecy inherent in such an assumption,
we
wanted to know exactly how deeply committed our women students were.
We
found that they were more deeply
committed than the men students. When
asked how they themselves felt
about having a career, 75 percent of the
women as against 60 percent
of the men were very favorable. When asked
"If you could have a
choice, would you choose to have a career at
all?" 92 percent of
the women compared to 81 percent of the men said
yes. So much for the
"women have no career commitment" myth.
|
Response
to: "How do you think these people feel
about your going
to (being in) graduate or professional
school?" by Sex
|
|
(Percent
Saying Person is "Very Favorable") |
|
Sex
of
respondent |
|
Person |
Male |
Female |
Male
Faculty |
47% |
32% |
Female
Faculty |
37% |
35% |
|
|
|
|
Response
to: "How do the following people feel
about your having
a career?" by Sex
|
|
(Percent
Saying Person is "Very
Favorable") |
|
Sex
of respondent |
|
Person |
Male |
Female |
Male
Faculty |
46% |
27% |
Female
Faculty |
35% |
27% |
|
|
|
|
|
|
There
is another myth which says that women
will drop out before completing
their degree. We had no way to test
what our subjects would do in the
future, but we could test how
strongly they felt about the possibility
of dropping out. We
discovered that 62 percent of the women said they
would be very
disappointed if they left school before completing their
education,
whereas only 53 percent of the men said this; 31.6 percent
of the men
said they were in school primarily to stay out of the draft.
Further
support comes from the external
environment, the general atmosphere and
mores of the society. We are
all aware that our society is more favorable
to men's getting advanced
education and having a career than to women's
doing these things. We
also know that we are influenced by these values.
But we had no real
way of measuring the amount or importance of this influence.
This
variable had to remain undetermined.
We
could, however, measure the perception by
our students of positive support
by specific people. We could
determine whether they felt their relatives,
friends, and spouses were
very favorable to their education and their
career. This was not
adequate to measure the total influence of the nonacademic
environment
in which our students lived, but it did give us some indicators.
Therefore we asked the same questions about the attitudes of these other
people as we had about the attitudes of the faculty.
Here,
too, the difference was quite apparent.
As seen in Tables III and IV,
men in all cases perceived more support
than women —in most cases considerably
more. The difference is greatest
of all on the question about careers.
This should not be surprising:
it is much more socially acceptable for
a woman to be well educated
than for her to earn money with that education.
TABLE
III
|
Response
to:
"How do you think these people feel about your going to (being
in) graduate or professional school?" |
|
|
Very
favorable |
Somewhat
favorable |
Not
too favorable |
Don't
know |
Yourself
|
M
W
|
66.9%
67.6%
|
24.1%
23.1%
|
6.1%
4.1%
|
2.9%
5.2%
|
Father
|
M
W
|
74.6%
47.5%
|
18.7%
37.0%
|
4.6%
6.1%
|
2.0%
6.1%
|
Mother
|
M
W
|
79.1%
49.3%
|
17.3%
33.4%
|
1.0%
10.9%
|
2.6%
6.4%
|
Siblings
|
M
W
|
52.7%
33.8%
|
31.3%
27.4%
|
1.5%
5.2%
|
14.5%
33.6%
|
Other
relatives
|
M
W
|
61.1%
20.8%
|
25.2%
30.6%
|
1.1%
13.7%
|
12.6%
34.9%
|
Most
friends opposite sex
|
M
W
|
43.4%
22.9%
|
37.6%
45.8%
|
1.7%
10.8%
|
17.3%
20.5%
|
Most
same-sex friends
|
M
W
|
42.9%
30.7%
|
39.9%
45.1%
|
2.5%
7.5%
|
14.8%
16.7%
|
Spouse,
boy- or girlfriend
|
M
W
|
64.5%
62.8%
|
26.4%
23.4%
|
2.7%
6.0%
|
6.5%
7.8%
|
Male
faculty member
|
M
W
|
46.7%
32.2%
|
20.0%
22.8%
|
2.1%
5.2%
|
31.3%
39.8%
|
Female
faculty member
|
M
W
|
37.1%
34.8%
|
15.8%
12.7%
|
0.9%
1.0%
|
46.2%
51.4%
|
Any
significant older people
|
M
W
|
56.2%
44.9%
|
19.1%
21.1%
|
1.4%
3.3%
|
23.3%
30.8%
|
TABLE
IV
|
Response
to: "How do the following people feel
about your having a career?"
by sex
|
|
|
Very
Favorable |
Somewhat
favorable |
No too
favorable |
Don't
know |
Yourself
|
M
W
|
60.3%
75.3%
|
24.1%
15.1%
|
11.1%
6.1%
|
4.4%
3.5%
|
Father
|
M
W
|
77.0%
47.5%
|
19.0%
37.0%
|
0.8%
6.1%
|
3.1%
9.4%
|
Mother
|
M
W
|
79.1%
17.3%
|
17.3%
33.4%
|
1.0%
10.9%
|
2.6%
6.4%
|
Siblings
|
M
W
|
52.7%
33.8%
|
31.3%
27.4%
|
1.5%
5.2%
|
14.5%
33.6%
|
Other
relatives
|
M
W
|
61.1%
20.8%
|
25.2%
30.6%
|
1.1%
13.7%
|
12.6%
34.9%
|
Most
friends
opposite sex |
M
W
|
43.4%
22.9%
|
37.6%
45.8%
|
1.7%
10.8%
|
17.3%
20.5%
|
Most
same- sex friends |
M
W
|
42.9%
30.7%
|
39.9%
45.1%
|
2.5%
7.5%
|
14.8%
16.7%
|
Spouse,
boy-or girlfriend |
M
W
|
62.0%
51.1%
|
29.4%
32.2%
|
2.1%
7.8%
|
6.5%
8.9%
|
Male
faculty member |
M
W
|
46.3%
26.6%
|
26.6%
30.6%
|
1.1%
1.9%
|
26.0%
41.0%
|
Female
faculty member |
M
W
|
35.3%
26.6%
|
22.3%
19.5%
|
0.7%
0.6%
|
41.6%
53.3%
|
Any
significant older people |
M
W
|
53.8%
35.5%
|
25.0%
28.1%
|
0.3%
1.8%
|
21.0%
34.6%
|
Much
more surprising was
a comparison of the attitudes of faculty, nonfaculty
and the students
themselves toward their graduate education and career.
First, for the
most part the weakest support of all came from the faculty
-- the very
people from whom students should have reason to expect the
strongest
support. Not only is this further confirmation of a tendency
toward a
null environment at the University of Chicago, but it implies
that it
is well within the faculty's power to significantly increase the
total
support students receive for going on with their education.
|
|
|
Second,
the men frequently reported
more favorable attitudes from others than
from themselves. This could
imply that the encouragement (or pressure)
they receive from parents,
spouses, and friends makes up for what they
do not receive from the
faculty. The women, by contrast, always report
a more favorable
attitude from themselves, with the difference most noticeable
in the
case of attitudes toward a career. Obviously women need their higher
levels of internal commitment. The cognitive dissonance between their
attitudes toward their own potential and the attitudes of others toward
it must create a stress endurable only with the help of a strong personal
determination to pursue both education and careers.
To
test our hypothesis further, we had to know
whether the faculty were openly
discouraging the students, male and
female, or whether their attitude
was, in fact, null. The tendency
toward the latter was confirmed by the
very low percentage of students
who felt that the faculty were noticeably
unfavorable to their
pursuing graduate education or careers and by the
very high percentage
who answered "Don't know." Among women
in particular, this
was the most frequent response to the two questions.
Among men,
however, "favorable," though low, had the highest
response
rate.
The
University of
Chicago is essentially a graduate and professional school
that prides
itself on its low student- faculty ratio. Even undergraduates,
less
than a third of the student body, are expected to attend mostly small
lectures and seminars and do a good deal of guided independent study.
Therefore we wanted to check the extent of student-faculty interaction
and the effect this interaction had on students. One way we did this was
by asking students whether a faculty member had ever revealed to them
his/her opinion about the students' seriousness, academic progress,
suitability
for the field of work, intellectual ability, and the other
concerns students
have about faculty estimations. From 43 to 93
percent of both women and
men students answered that not a single
faculty member had expressed an
opinion, or implied one, on any of
these matters. The intellectual environment
was indeed very
parched.
Yet
the overall picture
appeared more sterile for women than for men. The
number of
"don't know" responses given by women indicates that
not
only do they experience low levels of support, but they do not get
enough feedback from the faculty to know where they stand. The University
does not care enough about the women within it even to respond negatively.
Its discouragement is much more insidious: it fails to respond at
all.
TABLE
V |
Response
to: "Since you have been at the
University of Chicago, has any
faculty member in your department
(or collegiate division) ever told
you or given you the impression
that he thought ... ?" |
|
|
No
one
|
Yes,
one
faculty
member
|
Yes,more
than one
faculty member
|
That
you should apply for a scholarship or fellowship
|
M
F
|
55.8%
56.4%
|
18.8%
22.2%
|
25.4%
21.4%
|
That
the rate at which you are moving through the
program is too slow 30%
|
M
F
|
84.6%
88.4%
|
5.4%
9.7%
|
1.1%
2.7%
|
That
you are not working up to the
University's standards |
M
F
|
93.5%
88.4%
|
9.7%
8.9%
|
3.3%
2.7%
|
That
you should switch to another field |
M
F
|
47.9%
87.3%
|
19.4%
11.1%
|
1.1%
1.6%
|
That
you are well-suited for the field you are in |
M
F
|
89.8%
89.3%
|
7.5%
8.5%
|
2.7%
2.1%
|
You
are one of the best students in one of his classes or department
|
M
F
|
57.5%
59.8%
|
26.1%
27.6%
|
16.4%
12.6%
|
That
you write well |
M
F
|
52.9%
49.4%
|
24.4%
23.5%
|
22.7%
27.1%
|
|
|
|
This
discouragement by default manifests itself in many ways besides lack of
faculty encouragement for women students. The very structure of the University
is geared to meet the needs of men and those women whose lives most
closely
resemble men's. Since the life-styles of the population of
intellectually
qualified women are more heterogeneous and the demands
made upon them
more diverse than those of an intellectually similar
population of men,
fewer can comfortably fit into the University
environment.
The
two most
obvious examples of this are lack of child-care facilities and
lack of
female role models among the professors. The University will deliberately
keep a balance between younger and older faculty because some students
relate better to the former and some to the latter. It will also try to
have representatives from various fields within a discipline. But it sees
no need to provide a sexual representation. The result is that few women
have examples before them of how to be a female professional. The idea
of meeting the different needs of different students with different
backgrounds
is nothing new or radical. Traditionally the University
has provided,
within limits, fellowships, loans, and jobs for those
who need money;
housing for those who are married or are
undergraduates and can stand
dormitories; jobs for student wives
(though seldom for student husbands);
remedial courses for those whose
academic background is scanty; special
programs for those who find the
traditional ones too confining; sports
programs (primarily for men),
recreational facilities; health services
(usually without
gynecological care); and a host of other opportunities.
What
the University fails to acknowledge is
that all of these needs are met
within the confines of male standards.
Because most of academia is male
it has never stopped to consider that
what is good and necessary for most
men is not always good and
necessary for most women. Thus the University
proved very
understanding and flexible in its readiness to accommodate
men with
draft problems. It gave them draft-exempt jobs, letters of recommendation
and standing, showed them as enrolled in courses they were not taking
to preserve their deferments, and, if worst came to worst, held their
fellowships for them until they came back. But a pregnant woman will often
have to drop out -- or be forced to leave by losing her fellowship --
because the university feels no responsibility to provide child-care
centers.
Even when she does not leave school, or has her children
before she enters,
she usually has the major responsibility for their
care and this can be
quite detrimental to full academic
involvement.
This
sex-related difference was clearly evident among our students when they
were asked how children affected their academic work. Of all those who
were parents, 15 percent of the women and only 1 percent of the men said
children had a very unfavorable effect. Conversely, 16 percent of the
men and a predictable zero percent of the women felt children had a very
favorable effect. This is just one example of how the specific needs of
women students are not met because the needs of men students provide the
standards the university feels obligated to meet.
THE
EXTERNAL ENVIRONMENT
Most
important for our study, however, is the fact that students are presumed
to come from and exist in a supportive external environment. In reality,
that presumption is far more valid for most men than for most women. In
many different ways men have been expected and encouraged to go on with
their advanced education and need little mental effort to picture
themselves
in a professional role. Their parents, their undergraduate
professors,
their friends, their other role models, their spouses, all
contribute
to this environment. Even if they have neither positive
stimulus nor positive
goal, their draft boards (at the time of this
study) and the gnawing realization
that they will, after all, have to
do something with their lives, provides
a negative spur that is only
slightly less effective than a positive one.
For
women this is not the case. Instead, the
general social atmosphere in
which they function tends to work against
them. They learn to see women
who achieve in the traditional sense of
financial success and professional
advancement as deviants and
correctly perceive that such success often
costs more than it gains in
personal terms. Several research projects
(in addition to the Horner
research cited earlier) have documented what
most women have always
known. Beatrice Lipinski showed that women students
think of success
as something that is achieved by men4
but not by women. Others have shown that women are reluctant to violate
this social standard. As Kagan and Moss have noted, "The universe
of appropriate behaviors for males and females is delineated early in
development and it is difficult for the child to cross these culturally
given frontiers without considerable conflict and tension."5
These barriers account for Pierce's
finding that high achievement motivation
among high-school women
correlates much more closely with early marriage
than with success in
school.6 Those
women who do cross
the frontiers must, according to Maccoby, pay a high
price in anxiety
and "it is this anxiety which helps to account for
the lack of
productivity among those women who do make intellectual careers."
She feels that "this tells something of a horror story. It would
appear that even when a woman is suitably endowed intellectually and develops
the right temperament and habits of thought to make use of her
endowment,
she must be fleet of foot indeed to scale the hurdles
society has erected
for her and to remain a whole and happy person
while continuing to follow
her intellectual bent."7
Thus
it should not be surprising that women tend to aspire at a lower level
than men and to require even greater stimulus from the academic environment.
As Gropper and Fitzpatrick have pointed out, "Women appear to be
less influenced than men by their high grades in deciding in favor of
advanced education. But they are more influenced than men by their low
grades in deciding against advanced education."8
As a group, women are deprived of the
rich external environment of high
expectations and high encouragement
that research indicates is best for
personal growth and creative
production. Unless they have exceptional
backgrounds, they have little
to go on but their own internal commitment.
|
|
|
The
attrition rates alone testify
that this is rarely enough. During the sixties
women have earned roughly
53 percent of all high school diplomas, 36 percent
of all bachelor's
degrees, 31 percent of all master's degrees and 10 percent
of all
doctorates. The greatest drop-off is after the master's degree. Women
seem to lack the stimulus to go on to the doctorate. In retrospect, this
is a very logical response to the environment. It is socially expected for
women to graduate from high school, generally expected and always acceptable
for women to get a B.A. or a B.S., somewhat acceptable and occasionally
necessary for a woman to get a master's degree (since many of the
professions
in which women predominate require it). But Ph.D.'s and some
professional
degrees are considered unnecessary and often undesirable
for a woman to
obtain if she is to remain within the purview of social
acceptability. And,
given the job and salary inequities for holders of
such degrees, they are
not always economically useful.
The
University does little to remedy this
situation and much to exacerbate it.
It offers virtually no courses on
women and there is little material on
women in the regular courses. This
lack contributes to the feeling that
women are not worth studying. There
are few women on the faculty -- less
than half the percentage that
prevails in the Ph.D. pool from which the
University draws. These are
concentrated at the bottom of the academic ladder
or are off it
entirely, despite the fact that the percentage of women in
the older
Ph.D. pool is considerably larger than that in the younger and
that
women Ph.D.'s as a group have higher I.Q.'s, higher grade-point averages,
and higher class ranks than their male counterparts. This situation gives
many students the idea that high-quality academic women are rare. Some 40
percent of the women students in the SCOUW survey felt that the faculty
were less receptive to female students than to male students.
One
has only to look at the Rosenthal-Jacobson
experiments to see what a depressing
effect this environment can have on
the aspirations of women students. Although
they did not use sex as a
salient variable, these experimenters showed that
when teachers were
told that certain students (actually selected at random)
would perform
exceptionally well or poorly, those students altered their
normal
performance in the predicted direction to a significant degree. The
teachers stated that they were treating all students exactly alike, yet
investigation showed that they were subtly, unconsciously, encouraging or
discouraging the chosen ones.9
In
many ways this environment of subtle
discouragement by neglect is more pernicious
than a strongly negative
one would be. As Eric Berne has theorized, everyone
needs
"strokes"; and although good strokes are better than bad
strokes, bad strokes are better than none. In academic terms, this translates
that women will do better when they are pointedly told that their sex
makes
their abilities and their commitment suspect. At least overt
negative response
provides women with some interaction and some
standards by which they can
judge their behavior. It also creates a
challenge --something to be overcome.
if women are conscious of the
roadblocks they face as women at the University
and in society they are
in a better position to muster the energy to struggle
against them. They
not only know exactly what they have to face, but by
sharing their
struggle with other women they can create the context of emotional
support that every student needs for high achievement.
Historically
it is also evident that overt
opposition is preferable to motivational malnutrition.
Women did better
when things were worse. From the time they first pounded
on the doors of
higher education their progress was steadily upward until
thirty or
forty years ago. By then they no longer had to overcome the obstacle
of
disbelief, but at the same time they no longer had the internal stimulus
of knowing they were pioneers. Once women had made it, no one cared -- one
way or the other. Their place was still seen to be in the home, and the
graduate schools did little more than provide a few loopholes for the hardy.
The percentage of women earning Ph.D.'s began to go down in the 1920's and
has risen only slightly since its nadir in 1950.
In
summation, one can say that if the University
and the behavior of its faculty
do not directly discriminate against
women, they indirectly and insidiously
discriminate against them. The
University is less of an intellectual seedbed
than a psychological
gauntlet -- and it is one that the male students run
in full armor,
while the women students trip through in their bare skins.
Perhaps
the best analogy for understanding the
differentiating effect on men and
women of the null environment is to be
drawn from agriculture. If a farmer
transplants into a field two groups
of seedlings -- one having been nourished
thus far in rich, fertilized
loam and the other malnourished for having
struggled in desert sand --
and that farmer then tends all the seedlings
with virtually equal lack
of care, fertilizer, and water (perhaps favoring
the loam-grown
seedlings slightly because they look more promising), no
one should be
surprised if the lesser harvest is reaped from the desert-bred
plants.
Nor should we, with all the modern farm apparatus and information
available, shrug complacently and lament that there is no way to make the
desert bloom.
|
|
|
Copyright
(c) by Jo Freeman
1
The study was
directed by Ellen Fried, formerly of the National
Opinion Research
Center, assisted by Nancy Hartsock, then a graduate student
in
political science at the University of Chicago and now teaching at
the
Johns Hopkins University. The responses to the questionnaire have
been
published in Women in the University of Chicago (May 1970), available
from the Office of Public information, University of Chicago.
2
Marina
Horner, "Why Bright Women Fail," Psychology
Today,
November 1970.
3
Female faculty members scored identically to males, for instance
(see Table IV), in the percentage rated "very favorable" to
women graduate students' having a career, and only 2.6 percent higher
in "very favorable" attitudes toward women's being in graduate
or professional school.
4
Beatrice Lipinski,
Sex-Role Conflict and Achievement Motivation
in College Women,
unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Cincinnati,
1965.
5
Jerome
Kagan and Howard A. Moss, Birth to Maturity: A Study in
Psychological Development, (New York and London: Wiley 1962), p. 270.
6
James V.
Pierce, Sex Differences in Achievement Motivation of
Able High
School Students, Cooperative Research Project No. 1097,
University
of Chicago, Dec. 1961.
7
Eleanor Maccoby, "Women's
Intellect," in Farber and Wilson,
eds., The Potential of
Women, (New York: McGraw Hill, 1963).
8
G. L. Gropper and
Robert Fitzpatrick, Who Goes to Graduate School?,
(Pittsburgh:
American Institute for Research, 1959).
9
R. Rosenthal and L.
Jacobson, Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher
Expectations and
Pupils' Intellectual Development, (New York: Holt,
Rinehart,
1968).
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|